imotiv
Popular Posts
-
Patience is waiting. Not passively waiting. That is laziness. But to keep going when the going is hard and slow - that is patience.”
-
Attitude is a course with no graduation!
-
Today is our branch 34th anniversary. Hope everything planned will run smoothly.
-
A successful man is one who can lay a firm foundation with the bricks others have thrown at him.
Thursday, January 10, 2013
Psychology of chess
The Psychology of Chess
Dr. Fernand Gobet is a professor of Cognitive Psychology and an International Master. He is a former Swiss Junior Champion and Swiss Champion, and was co-editor of the Swiss Chess Review from 1981 to 1989. In 1992, he wrote his Ph.D. dissertation on the memories of a chess player. He has written many books and articles about chess and psychology. He has been studying many aspects of chess psychology such as mental imagery, pattern recognition, and study and playing patterns of chess players.
After studying hundreds of chess players, Gobet has found a strong correlation between the number of hours chess players have dedicated to chess (deliberate practice) and their current rating. In one study of 104 players (101 males and 3 females), including 39 untitled players without any rating, 39 untitled players with ratings, 13 FIDE masters (FM), 10 International Masters (IM), and 3 GMs, he found that the unrated players reported and average of 8,303 hours of dedication to chess; the rated, but untitled players reported 11,715 hours; the FMs reported 19,618 hours and the IMs reported 27,929 hours (no information on GMs). It took an average of 11,000 hours to reach 2200. One player needed around 3,000 hours to reach 2200, while another player spent more than 23,000 hours to achieve the same level.
The average master (rated 2257) had 7.0 years of serious practice. The average expert (2174) had 1.03 years of serious practice. The masters increased their rating an average of 7 Elo (FIDE) points per year of serious practice, whereas the experts only increased their rating an average of 1 Elo point per year of serious practice. Experts increased their chess-playing skill level very little with time, whereas masters kept increasing theirs.
In Gobet’s survey, 83% of the players reported playing blitz, 80% had a coach at some point, 67% used databases (game databases, but not playing programs), 66% played against chess programs; 56% followed chess games without using a chessboard, 23% played blindfold games. Stronger players were more likely to have a coach, use databases, and played blitz.
Stronger players also tended to own more chess books (and read them) than weaker players. As an individual activity, reading chess books were the most important predictor of chess skill. For group activity, coaching and speed games were the most significant predictors of chess skill, but less a predictor with age.
Dr. Gobet also found that group practice (including tournament games) was a better predictor of high-level performance than individual practice.
It has been shown that non-professional players who started playing chess at a young age, show interest and commitment to chess until the late teens. This is when the amount of time devoted to chess peaks (about age 18). After this, players start work or attend university and/or get married, which reduces the time spent playing chess. By the mid-30s, when family and work issues are more stable, non-professional chess players return to the game and play more frequently.
Gobet showed that there was a clear indication that the first three years of serious chess practice at early ages are much more advantageous than the first three years of serious practice at later ages. Most masters became serious about chess between 10 and 12. Most experts became serious about chess around 14.
One important role in chess skill is pattern recognition (vs. the ability to search through the problem space). Through years of practice and study, masters have learned several hundred thousands of perceptual chess patterns (called chunking). When one of these patterns is recognized in a particular position, the master then has rapid access to information such as potential moves or move sequences, tactics, and strategies. This explains automatic and intuitive discovery of good moves by a master, as well as extraordinary memory for game-like chess positions.
Search functions at a chess board, including the number of candidate moves visited and the depth of search, may not differ between masters and amateurs, according to Adrian de Groot (1914-2006), a Dutch chess master and psychologist. His findings were that Grandmasters do not search reliable deeper than amateurs. However, other studies (Holding 1989) show that strong players really do search deeper than weaker players. Holding argued that de Groot’s experiment wasn’t good enough to detect existing differences between Grandmasters and amateurs.
In a classic study of chess visualization by de Groot at the University of Amsterdam, findings show that, on average, chess masters calculated no deeper than weaker players, and often examined fewer chess variations. However, the master almost always selected superior moves.
De Groot found that there were four stages in the task of choosing a move. The first stage was the orientation phase, in which the player assessed the chess situation and determined a very general idea of what to do next. The second stage was the exploration phase, in which the player examined in his head some branches of the game tree. The third phase was the investigation phase, in which the player chose a probable best move. The fourth phase was the proof phase, in which the player confirmed that the results of his investigation of the best move were valid.
In 1990, Pertti Saariluoma studied the search function of top players and suggested the International Masters and Grandmasters sometimes search less than master chess players. In tactical positions, he found that masters with a 2200 Elo rating looked at 52 nodes and at the largest depth of 5.1 moves. By comparison, the IM and GM searched, on average, 23 nodes with an average depth of 3.6 moves.
Saarilouma showed that attention is an important notion in chess because chess players must be able to detect various kinds of possibilities and threats. Inattention over one move could destroy hours of good work in a chess game. This means that understanding a chess player’s information processing attention is a central topic in the psychology of chess. Masters are superior than average players in picking up information from a board position. They perceive faster all kinds of chess-specific perceptual cues such as whether one of the kings is checked or not, or if there is a mate in one move.
Data from speed chess and simultaneous chess, show that limitations in thinking time do not impair chess master performance. Chess masters seem to be more highly selective of their moves and direct their attention rapidly to good moves. Grandmasters do not look at a lot of continuations of the game before choosing a move. It seems that chunking, recognition of known chess patterns, plays a key role in a master’s ability to play fast and accurate.
So do strong players rely more on analyzing various alternatives, or do they rely on recognizing familiar chess patterns in the situation? Do chess players put most of their emphasis on their analytic skills or on building up a huge knowledge base in their heads? Perhaps it is a combination of search skills and pattern recognition.
In 1986, Gobet tried to replicate de Groot’s 1946 experiment of Grandmaster vs. amateur examination of chess positions. Gobet was able to test four IMs, eight masters, and a total of 48 Swiss chess players on a series of chess quizzes in which the goal was to find the best move for White, without moving the pieces, with thinking time limited to 30 minutes.
Both pattern recognition and search models predict that strong players choose better moves, that they select moves faster, and that they generate more nodes in one minute. Gobet showed that the first prediction was met, but the second and third were supported only weakly. Search models predict that strong players search more nodes and search deeper. The first prediction was not met, but the second was in that the difference lies in the average depth of search, not in the maximal depth of search. Finally, pattern recognition models predict that strong players mention fewer base moves, reinvestigate more often the same move, and jump less often between different moves. All these predictions were met.
Gobet showed that another possible predictor of chess skill might be the starting age. The average age at which players of each group started playing seriously was the following: non-rated players – 18.6 years; rated players – 14.2 years; FMs – 11.6 years; IMs – 10.3 years; GMs (small sample) – 11.3 years. Almost all the players with titles started playing seriously no later than age 12.
Becoming a master requires training activities that go beyond the type of repetitive and feedback-informed activities typically emphasized in earlier days. Chess theory and computer technology has changed the ways chess players prepare for their games. Masters try to memorize opening variations with the aid of chess databases, they investigate opening positions to find novelties to surprise their opponents, and they play tournament or training games against other players, or on the Internet, or against strong chess computer programs.
Dr. Gobet has also looked into the personalities of chess players. Studies have found that adult chess players are more introverted and intuitive than the general population. However, it is the more energetic and extroverted children that are more likely to play chess. These children are, in general, more likely to try out activities such as chess than their less extroverted peers. Children players who were stronger in chess than their peers were more curious, had broader intellectual and cultural interests, and were more accomplished in school than children who were weaker chess players.
In addition, stronger players also tend to me more intuitive than weaker ones. Chess players also scored higher than non-players on the measures of orderliness and unconventional thinking.
Another consideration in chess thinking is the effect of aging among chess players. Studies have shown that in memory tasks where positions are briefly presented, for the same skill level, younger players recalled chess positions better than older players. In spite of producing worse performance than younger players of the same skill level in memory tasks, older players performed equally well in problem solving tasks where they had to choose the best move, and that they were also faster at choosing their move.
In 1894, Alfred Binet (1857-1911), a French psychologist, carried out the first study on the mental abilities of chess masters. In 1903, he was the first psychologist to develop an intelligence test. He devised the Intelligence Quotient (IQ) tests, where the intelligence score was the quotient of mental age to physical age.
Binet had done a number of experiments to see how well chess players played without looking at the chess board (known as blindfold chess). He found that only some of the master chess players could play from memory, and a few could play many games simultaneously without looking at the chess boards. To remember the positions of the pieces on the boards, some chess players envisioned exact replicas of specific chess sets. Other players envisioned an abstract schema of the game.
In 1927, three Russian psychologists (Djakow, Petrowski and Rudik) studied eight of the best grandmasters of the time. The players included Emanuel Lasker, Richard Reti, Savielly Tartakower, Carlos Torre, Peter Romanovsky, Ernst Gurenfeld, and Rudolf Spielmann. They did not find any differences with a control sample on general intelligence or visuo-spatial memory, with the exception of memory tasks where the material to be recalled was closely related to chess.
After a century of investigation, not a single study with adult chess players has managed to establish a link between chess skill and intelligence. Intellect had little predictive power among strong chess players.
Another hypothesis states that people who become strong chess players have exceptional intelligence and/or memory. However, there is little evidence to support this viewpoint. Most researchers have found minimal correlations between measures of IQ and official chess ratings. What researchers have found out is that many top grandmasters have a very good memory when it comes to chess. They can recall a large number of games, move-b-move, and they can reproduce practical chess positions with very few mistakes on a new board and set. However, if the position is random, with chess pieces just dropped on a chess board, the master cannot reproduce the position any better than an amateur. General intelligence and memory by themselves do not appear to distinguish strong chess players from ordinary ones.
Does all this matter? Well, to the best players in the world, perhaps not. It was Bobby Fischer who said, “I don’t believe in psychology. I believe in good moves.”
–Bill Wall
Be Sociable, Share!
inShare
1
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment